第9行: |
第9行: |
| Lamarck's theory of evolution, Darwinian evolution, and Waddington's genetic assimilation. All the theories offer explanations of how organisms respond to a changed environment with adaptive inherited change.]] | | Lamarck's theory of evolution, Darwinian evolution, and Waddington's genetic assimilation. All the theories offer explanations of how organisms respond to a changed environment with adaptive inherited change.]] |
| | | |
− | 拉马克的进化论、达尔文进化论和华丁顿的基因同化论。[参考译文]所有的理论都解释了有机体是如何通过适应性遗传变化来应对环境变化的
| + | 拉马克的进化论、达尔文进化论和华丁顿的遗传同化论。[参考译文]所有的理论都解释了有机体是如何通过适应性遗传变化来应对环境变化的 |
| + | --~英文wiki无此段 |
| | | |
| In [[evolutionary biology]], the '''Baldwin effect''' describes the effect of learned behavior on evolution. In brief, [[James Mark Baldwin]] and others suggested during the [[eclipse of Darwinism]] in the late 19th century that an organism's ''ability to learn'' new behaviors (e.g. to acclimatise to a new stressor) will affect its reproductive success and will therefore have an effect on the genetic makeup of its species through [[natural selection]]. Though this process appears similar to [[Lamarckism|Lamarckian evolution]], Lamarck proposed that living things ''inherited'' their parents' acquired characteristics. The Baldwin effect has been independently proposed several times, and today it is generally recognized as part of the [[Modern synthesis (20th century)|modern synthesis]]. | | In [[evolutionary biology]], the '''Baldwin effect''' describes the effect of learned behavior on evolution. In brief, [[James Mark Baldwin]] and others suggested during the [[eclipse of Darwinism]] in the late 19th century that an organism's ''ability to learn'' new behaviors (e.g. to acclimatise to a new stressor) will affect its reproductive success and will therefore have an effect on the genetic makeup of its species through [[natural selection]]. Though this process appears similar to [[Lamarckism|Lamarckian evolution]], Lamarck proposed that living things ''inherited'' their parents' acquired characteristics. The Baldwin effect has been independently proposed several times, and today it is generally recognized as part of the [[Modern synthesis (20th century)|modern synthesis]]. |
第15行: |
第16行: |
| In evolutionary biology, the Baldwin effect describes the effect of learned behavior on evolution. In brief, James Mark Baldwin and others suggested during the eclipse of Darwinism in the late 19th century that an organism's ability to learn new behaviors (e.g. to acclimatise to a new stressor) will affect its reproductive success and will therefore have an effect on the genetic makeup of its species through natural selection. Though this process appears similar to Lamarckian evolution, Lamarck proposed that living things inherited their parents' acquired characteristics. The Baldwin effect has been independently proposed several times, and today it is generally recognized as part of the modern synthesis. | | In evolutionary biology, the Baldwin effect describes the effect of learned behavior on evolution. In brief, James Mark Baldwin and others suggested during the eclipse of Darwinism in the late 19th century that an organism's ability to learn new behaviors (e.g. to acclimatise to a new stressor) will affect its reproductive success and will therefore have an effect on the genetic makeup of its species through natural selection. Though this process appears similar to Lamarckian evolution, Lamarck proposed that living things inherited their parents' acquired characteristics. The Baldwin effect has been independently proposed several times, and today it is generally recognized as part of the modern synthesis. |
| | | |
− | 在进化生物学中,鲍德温效应描述了学习行为对进化的影响。简而言之,在19世纪末达尔文主义的日蚀期间,詹姆斯·马克·鲍德温和其他人提出,生物体学习新行为的能力(例如:。为了适应新的压力)会影响它的繁殖成功,因此会通过自然选择影响其物种的基因组成。虽然这个过程与拉马克的进化过程相似,但拉马克认为生物继承了他们父母获得的特征。鲍德温效应已经被独立地提出了好几次,今天它被普遍认为是现代综合的一部分。
| + | 在'''演化生物学 Evolutionary Biology'''中,'''鲍德温效应 Baldwin Effect'''描述了习得行为对进化的影响。简而言之,在19世纪末'''达尔文主义 Darwinism'''的衰落期间,'''詹姆斯·马克·鲍德温 James Mark Baldwin'''及其他一些人提出,生物体学习新行为的能力(如为了适应新的压力)会影响其繁殖,进而会通过'''自然选择 Natural Selection'''影响其物种的基因组成。这个过程似乎与'''拉马克主义 Lamarckism'''中相似,但拉马克认为生物继承了其父母后天获得的特性。鲍德温效应已独立提出过多次,如今普遍认为其为'''现代进化综论 Modern Evolutionary Synthesis'''的一部分。 |
| | | |
| | | |
| | | |
− | =="A New Factor in Evolution"== | + | =="A New Factor in Evolution" “进化的新因素”== |
| | | |
| The effect, then unnamed, was put forward in 1896 in a paper "A New Factor in Evolution" by [[United States|American]] [[psychology|psychologist]] [[James Mark Baldwin]]. The paper proposed a mechanism for specific selection for general learning ability. As [[Robert J. Richards|Robert Richards]] explains:<ref name=Richards1987>{{cite book | author=Richards, Robert J. | year=1987 | title=Darwin and the Emergence of Evolutionary Theories of Mind and Behavior| publisher=The University of Chicago Press |location=Chicago | isbn=978-0-226-71199-7 | page=399| author-link=Robert J. Richards }}</ref> | | The effect, then unnamed, was put forward in 1896 in a paper "A New Factor in Evolution" by [[United States|American]] [[psychology|psychologist]] [[James Mark Baldwin]]. The paper proposed a mechanism for specific selection for general learning ability. As [[Robert J. Richards|Robert Richards]] explains:<ref name=Richards1987>{{cite book | author=Richards, Robert J. | year=1987 | title=Darwin and the Emergence of Evolutionary Theories of Mind and Behavior| publisher=The University of Chicago Press |location=Chicago | isbn=978-0-226-71199-7 | page=399| author-link=Robert J. Richards }}</ref> |
第25行: |
第26行: |
| The effect, then unnamed, was put forward in 1896 in a paper "A New Factor in Evolution" by American psychologist James Mark Baldwin. The paper proposed a mechanism for specific selection for general learning ability. As Robert Richards explains: | | The effect, then unnamed, was put forward in 1896 in a paper "A New Factor in Evolution" by American psychologist James Mark Baldwin. The paper proposed a mechanism for specific selection for general learning ability. As Robert Richards explains: |
| | | |
− | 1896年,美国心理学家詹姆斯·马克·鲍德温在一篇名为《进化的新因素》的论文中提出了这一效应。本文提出了一种针对一般学习能力的具体选择机制。正如罗伯特 · 理查兹解释的那样:
| + | 1896年,美国心理学家詹姆斯·马克·鲍德温在一篇名为《进化的新因素》的论文中提出了这一效应(1987年又发表了第二篇论文)。该文提出了一种针对一般学习能力的具体选择机制。正如'''罗伯特·理查兹 Robert Richards'''解释的那样: |
| | | |
| | | |
第39行: |
第40行: |
| If animals entered a new environment—or their old environment rapidly changed—those | | If animals entered a new environment—or their old environment rapidly changed—those |
| | | |
− | 如果动物进入一个新的环境,或者它们原来的环境迅速发生变化,那么
| + | |
| | | |
| that could flexibly respond by learning new behaviors or by ontogenetically adapting would | | that could flexibly respond by learning new behaviors or by ontogenetically adapting would |
第45行: |
第46行: |
| that could flexibly respond by learning new behaviors or by ontogenetically adapting would | | that could flexibly respond by learning new behaviors or by ontogenetically adapting would |
| | | |
− | 可以通过学习新的行为或者通过个体遗传适应灵活地作出反应
| + | |
| | | |
| be naturally preserved. This saved remnant would, over several generations, have the | | be naturally preserved. This saved remnant would, over several generations, have the |
第51行: |
第52行: |
| be naturally preserved. This saved remnant would, over several generations, have the | | be naturally preserved. This saved remnant would, over several generations, have the |
| | | |
− | 自然保存。这些被保存下来的残余物,经过几代后,会有
| + | |
| | | |
| opportunity to exhibit spontaneously congenital variations similar to their acquired traits and | | opportunity to exhibit spontaneously congenital variations similar to their acquired traits and |
第57行: |
第58行: |
| opportunity to exhibit spontaneously congenital variations similar to their acquired traits and | | opportunity to exhibit spontaneously congenital variations similar to their acquired traits and |
| | | |
− | 有机会表现出自发的先天性变异,类似于他们获得的特征和
| + | |
| | | |
| have these variations naturally selected. It would look as though the acquired traits had sunk | | have these variations naturally selected. It would look as though the acquired traits had sunk |
第63行: |
第64行: |
| have these variations naturally selected. It would look as though the acquired traits had sunk | | have these variations naturally selected. It would look as though the acquired traits had sunk |
| | | |
− | 这些变化是自然选择的。这看起来好像后天获得的特征已经消失了
| + | |
| | | |
| into the hereditary substance in a Lamarckian fashion, but the process would really be [[Neo-Darwinism|neo-Darwinian]]. | | into the hereditary substance in a Lamarckian fashion, but the process would really be [[Neo-Darwinism|neo-Darwinian]]. |
第69行: |
第70行: |
| into the hereditary substance in a Lamarckian fashion, but the process would really be neo-Darwinian. | | into the hereditary substance in a Lamarckian fashion, but the process would really be neo-Darwinian. |
| | | |
− | 以拉马克的方式将其转化为遗传物质,但这个过程实际上是新达尔文主义的。
| + | 如果动物处于新环境或其原来所处的环境急剧变化,那些能够通过学习新行为或自身基因适应来灵活作出反应的动物会自然地留存下来。这些幸存者,经过几代之后,有机会自发地表现出与他们获得的特征相似的先天变异,并让其经自然选择而留存。看上去获得性特征以拉马克的方式留在了遗传物质上,但这个过程实际上是'''新达尔文主义 Neo-Darwinism'''的。 |
| | | |
| </blockquote> | | </blockquote> |
第83行: |
第84行: |
| Selected offspring would tend to have an increased capacity for learning new skills rather than being confined to genetically coded, relatively fixed abilities. In effect, it places emphasis on the fact that the sustained behavior of a species or group can shape the evolution of that species. The "Baldwin effect" is better understood in evolutionary developmental biology literature as a scenario in which a character or trait change occurring in an organism as a result of its interaction with its environment becomes gradually assimilated into its developmental genetic or epigenetic repertoire (Simpson, 1953; Newman, 2002). In the words of Daniel Dennett, | | Selected offspring would tend to have an increased capacity for learning new skills rather than being confined to genetically coded, relatively fixed abilities. In effect, it places emphasis on the fact that the sustained behavior of a species or group can shape the evolution of that species. The "Baldwin effect" is better understood in evolutionary developmental biology literature as a scenario in which a character or trait change occurring in an organism as a result of its interaction with its environment becomes gradually assimilated into its developmental genetic or epigenetic repertoire (Simpson, 1953; Newman, 2002). In the words of Daniel Dennett, |
| | | |
− | 被选中的后代往往具有更强的学习新技能的能力,而不是局限于基因编码,相对固定的能力。实际上,它强调了一个事实,即一个物种或群体的持续行为可以塑造该物种的进化。在演化发育生物学文学中,“ Baldwin 效应”被更好地理解为一个场景,在这个场景中,一个有机体由于与其环境的相互作用而发生的性格或特征变化逐渐被同化到其发育的遗传或表观遗传剧目中(Simpson,1953; Newman,2002)。用丹尼尔 · 丹尼特的话来说,
| + | 被选择留存的后代往往具有更强的学习新技能的能力,而不仅局限于基因编码的相对固定的能力。实际上,它强调以下事实:一个物种或群体的持续行为可以影响其进化。在'''演化发育生物学 Evolutionary developmental biology'''的文献中,“鲍德温效应”得到了更好的理解——生物体由于与其环境的相互作用而发生的的特征或性状变化逐渐被同化到其发育遗传或表观遗传库中。用'''丹尼尔·丹尼特 Daniel Dennett'''的话来说, |
| | | |
| | | |
第97行: |
第98行: |
| Thanks to the Baldwin effect, species can be said to pretest the efficacy of particular different designs by phenotypic (individual) exploration of the space of nearby possibilities. If a particularly winning setting is thereby discovered, this discovery will create a new selection pressure: organisms that are closer in the adaptive landscape to that discovery will have a clear advantage over those more distant. | | Thanks to the Baldwin effect, species can be said to pretest the efficacy of particular different designs by phenotypic (individual) exploration of the space of nearby possibilities. If a particularly winning setting is thereby discovered, this discovery will create a new selection pressure: organisms that are closer in the adaptive landscape to that discovery will have a clear advantage over those more distant. |
| | | |
− | 由于鲍德温效应,可以说物种通过表型(个体)探索附近空间的可能性,预先测试了特定不同设计的功效。如果一个特别成功的环境被发现,这个发现将创造一个新的选择压力: 在适应环境中更接近这个发现的有机体将比那些更遥远的有机体有明显的优势。
| + | 由于鲍德温效应,可以说物种通过表型(个体)探索附近可能性的空间,预先测试了特定不同设计的功效。若由此发现一个特别适宜的环境,该发现将导致新的选择压力: 在适应环境中,更接近此发现的生物体将较其他生物体有明显的优势。 |
| | | |
| </blockquote> | | </blockquote> |
第111行: |
第112行: |
| An update to the Baldwin effect was developed by Jean Piaget, Paul Weiss, and Conrad Waddington in the 1960s–1970s. This new version included an explicit role for the social in shaping subsequent natural change in humans (both evolutionary and developmental), with reference to alterations of selection pressures. | | An update to the Baldwin effect was developed by Jean Piaget, Paul Weiss, and Conrad Waddington in the 1960s–1970s. This new version included an explicit role for the social in shaping subsequent natural change in humans (both evolutionary and developmental), with reference to alterations of selection pressures. |
| | | |
− | 1960-1970年代,让 · 皮亚杰、保罗 · 维斯和康拉德 · 华丁顿对鲍德温效应进行了更新。这个新的版本包括了社会在塑造人类随后的自然变化(包括进化和发展)中的明确作用,并提到了选择压力的改变。 | + | 1960-1970年代,'''让·皮亚杰 Jean Piaget'''、'''保罗·维斯 Paul Weiss'''和'''康拉德·华丁顿 Conrad Waddington'''修正了鲍德温效应。该新版本明确了社会在塑造人类随后的自然变化(包括进化和发展)中的作用,并提到了选择压力的改变。 |
| | | |
| | | |
第119行: |
第120行: |
| As is to be expected from Stigler's law, subsequent research shows that Baldwin was not the first to identify the process; Douglas Spalding mentioned it in 1873. | | As is to be expected from Stigler's law, subsequent research shows that Baldwin was not the first to identify the process; Douglas Spalding mentioned it in 1873. |
| | | |
− | 正如斯蒂格勒定律所预期的那样,随后的研究表明,Baldwin 并不是第一个发现这个过程的人; 道格拉斯·斯普拉丁在1873年提到过这个过程。
| + | 正如'''斯蒂格勒定律 Stigler's Law'''所指,随后的研究表明,鲍德温并不是第一个发现这个过程的人; '''道格拉斯·斯波尔丁 Douglas Spalding'''在1873年提到过这个过程。 |
| | | |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==Examples== | + | ==Examples 举例== |
| | | |
| Suppose a species is threatened by a new [[predator]] and there is a behavior that makes it more difficult for the predator to kill individuals of the species. Individuals who learn the behavior more quickly will obviously be at an advantage. As time goes on, the ability to learn the behavior will improve (by genetic selection), and at some point it will seem to be an [[instinct]]. Baldwin gives the following case involving cooperation: "Animals may be kept alive let us say in a given environment by social cooperation only; these transmit this social type of variation to posterity; thus social adaptation sets the direction of physical phylogeny and physical heredity is determined in part by this factor" (Baldwin, 1896, p. 553). | | Suppose a species is threatened by a new [[predator]] and there is a behavior that makes it more difficult for the predator to kill individuals of the species. Individuals who learn the behavior more quickly will obviously be at an advantage. As time goes on, the ability to learn the behavior will improve (by genetic selection), and at some point it will seem to be an [[instinct]]. Baldwin gives the following case involving cooperation: "Animals may be kept alive let us say in a given environment by social cooperation only; these transmit this social type of variation to posterity; thus social adaptation sets the direction of physical phylogeny and physical heredity is determined in part by this factor" (Baldwin, 1896, p. 553). |
第141行: |
第142行: |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==Controversy and acceptance== | + | ==Controversy and acceptance 争议和接受== |
| | | |
| | | |
第149行: |
第150行: |
| Initially Baldwin's ideas were not incompatible with the prevailing, but uncertain, ideas about the mechanism of transmission of hereditary information and at least two other biologists put forward very similar ideas in 1896. In 1901, Maurice Maeterlinck referred to behavioural adaptations to prevailing climates in different species of bees as ‘what had merely been an idea, therefore, and opposed to instinct, has thus by slow degrees become an instinctive habit’. The Baldwin effect theory subsequently became more controversial, with scholars being split between "Baldwin boosters" and "Baldwin skeptics". The theory was first called the "Baldwin effect" by George Gaylord Simpson in 1953. In 1942, the evolutionary biologist Julian Huxley promoted the Baldwin effect as part of the modern synthesis, saying the concept had been unduly neglected by evolutionists. | | Initially Baldwin's ideas were not incompatible with the prevailing, but uncertain, ideas about the mechanism of transmission of hereditary information and at least two other biologists put forward very similar ideas in 1896. In 1901, Maurice Maeterlinck referred to behavioural adaptations to prevailing climates in different species of bees as ‘what had merely been an idea, therefore, and opposed to instinct, has thus by slow degrees become an instinctive habit’. The Baldwin effect theory subsequently became more controversial, with scholars being split between "Baldwin boosters" and "Baldwin skeptics". The theory was first called the "Baldwin effect" by George Gaylord Simpson in 1953. In 1942, the evolutionary biologist Julian Huxley promoted the Baldwin effect as part of the modern synthesis, saying the concept had been unduly neglected by evolutionists. |
| | | |
− | 最初,鲍德温的想法并不与盛行的,但不确定的,关于遗传信息传递机制的想法不相容,至少其他两个生物学家在1896年提出了非常相似的想法。1901年,莫里斯·梅特林克提到不同种类的蜜蜂对当时气候的行为适应性,称之为仅仅是一种想法,因此,与本能相反,慢慢地变成了一种本能的习惯。鲍德温效应理论随后变得更具争议性,学者们被分为“鲍德温支持者”和“鲍德温怀疑论者”。这个理论在1953年首次被乔治·盖洛德·辛普森称为“ Baldwin 效应”。1942年,进化生物学家朱利安 · 赫胥黎宣称鲍德温效应是现代综合论的一部分,说这个概念被进化论者过度忽视了。
| + | 最初,鲍德温的观点与广泛流传但未加验证的关于遗传信息传递机制的观点并不矛盾,且在1896年至少有两位生物学家提出了非常相似的观点。1901年,'''莫里斯·梅特林克 Maurice Maeterlinck'''提到不同种类的蜜蜂对当时气候的行为适应性,称其之前仅仅是一种想法,因此,与本能相反,慢慢地变成了一种本能的习惯。鲍德温效应理论随后变得更具争议性,学者们分为“鲍德温支持者”和“鲍德温怀疑论者”。1953年'''乔治·盖洛德·辛普森 George Gaylord Simpson'''首次命名该理论为“鲍德温效应”。1942年,进化生物学家'''朱利安·赫胥黎 Julian Huxley'''将鲍德温效应作为现代进化综论的一部分加以推广,称进化论者过度忽视了这一概念。 |
| | | |
| | | |
第157行: |
第158行: |
| In the 1960s, the evolutionary biologist Ernst Mayr contended that the Baldwin effect theory was untenable because | | In the 1960s, the evolutionary biologist Ernst Mayr contended that the Baldwin effect theory was untenable because |
| | | |
− | 在20世纪60年代,进化生物学家恩斯特 · 迈尔声称鲍德温效应理论是站不住脚的,因为
| + | 在20世纪60年代,进化生物学家'''恩斯特·迈尔'''称鲍德温效应理论是站不住脚的,因为 |
| | | |
| # the argument is stated in terms of the individual genotype, whereas what is really exposed to the selection pressure is a phenotypically and genetically variable population; | | # the argument is stated in terms of the individual genotype, whereas what is really exposed to the selection pressure is a phenotypically and genetically variable population; |
第175行: |
第176行: |
| it is assumed that phenotypic rigidity is selectively superior to phenotypic flexibility. | | it is assumed that phenotypic rigidity is selectively superior to phenotypic flexibility. |
| | | |
− | 假定表型刚性有选择地优于表型柔性。
| + | 假定表型刚性有选择性优于表型柔性。 |
| | | |
| | | |
第183行: |
第184行: |
| In 1987 Geoffrey Hinton and Steven Nowlan demonstrated by computer simulation that learning can accelerate evolution, and they associated this with the Baldwin effect. | | In 1987 Geoffrey Hinton and Steven Nowlan demonstrated by computer simulation that learning can accelerate evolution, and they associated this with the Baldwin effect. |
| | | |
− | 1987年,Geoffrey Hinton 和 Steven Nowlan 通过计算机模拟证明了学习可以加速进化,他们将其与 Baldwin 效应联系起来。
| + | 1987年,'''杰弗里·辛顿 Geoffrey Hinton''' 和 '''史蒂芬·诺兰 Steven Nowlan''' 通过计算机模拟证明了学习可以加速进化,他们将此与鲍德温效应联系起来。 |
| | | |
| | | |
第191行: |
第192行: |
| Paul Griffiths suggests two reasons for the continuing interest in the Baldwin effect. The first is the role mind is understood to play in the effect. The second is the connection between development and evolution in the effect. Baldwin’s account of how neurophysiological and conscious mental factors may contribute to the effect brings into focus the question of the possible survival value of consciousness. | | Paul Griffiths suggests two reasons for the continuing interest in the Baldwin effect. The first is the role mind is understood to play in the effect. The second is the connection between development and evolution in the effect. Baldwin’s account of how neurophysiological and conscious mental factors may contribute to the effect brings into focus the question of the possible survival value of consciousness. |
| | | |
− | 保罗 · 格里菲斯提出了人们对鲍德温效应持续兴趣的两个原因。首先,我们可以理解心智在这种效果中所扮演的角色。二是发展与演变的关系。鲍德温关于神经生理学和有意识的心理因素如何促成这种效应的论述,使人们更加关注意识可能的生存价值问题。
| + | '''保罗·格里菲斯 Paul Griffiths'''指出了人们对鲍德温效应持续兴趣的两个原因。首先,我们可以理解心智在这种效果中所扮演的角色。二是其中发展与演变的关系。鲍德温关于神经生理学和意识心理因素如何促成这种效应的解释,使人们更加关注意识可能的存在价值问题。 |
| | | |
| | | |
第199行: |
第200行: |
| The [[house finch's colonisation of North America has provided empirical evidence of the Baldwin effect.]] | | The [[house finch's colonisation of North America has provided empirical evidence of the Baldwin effect.]] |
| | | |
− | [雀鸟在北美的殖民活动提供了鲍德温效应的经验证明。] | + | [图一:家朱雀在北美的定殖提供了鲍德温效应的经验证明。] |
| | | |
| | | |
第207行: |
第208行: |
| Still, observes David Depew, "it is striking that a rather diverse lot of contemporary evolutionary theorists, most of whom regard themselves as supporters of the Modern Synthesis, have of late become 'Baldwin boosters These Baldwin boosters | | Still, observes David Depew, "it is striking that a rather diverse lot of contemporary evolutionary theorists, most of whom regard themselves as supporters of the Modern Synthesis, have of late become 'Baldwin boosters These Baldwin boosters |
| | | |
− | 不过,戴维•迪普(David Depew)评论道: “令人惊讶的是,许多不同的当代进化论理论家,其中大多数人自认为是现代综合论(Modern Synthesis)的支持者,最近却成为了‘鲍德温的支持者,这些鲍德温
| + | |
| | | |
| | | |
第221行: |
第222行: |
| are typically evolutionary psychologists who are searching for scenarios in which a population can get itself by behavioral trial and error onto a "hard to find" part of the fitness landscape in which human brain, language, and mind can rapidly coevolve. They are searching for what Daniel Dennett, himself a Baldwin booster, calls an "evolutionary crane," an instrument to do some heavy lifting fast. | | are typically evolutionary psychologists who are searching for scenarios in which a population can get itself by behavioral trial and error onto a "hard to find" part of the fitness landscape in which human brain, language, and mind can rapidly coevolve. They are searching for what Daniel Dennett, himself a Baldwin booster, calls an "evolutionary crane," an instrument to do some heavy lifting fast. |
| | | |
− | 是典型的进化心理学家,他们正在寻找一个场景,在这个场景中,一个种群可以通过行为试验和错误到一个“难以找到”的适应景观的一部分,在这个景观中,人类的大脑,语言和思维可以迅速共同。他们正在寻找一种被丹尼尔 · 丹尼特称为“进化起重机”的工具,丹尼尔 · 丹尼特自己就是鲍德温的支持者。
| + | 不过,'''戴维•迪皮尤 David Depew'''评论道: “令人惊讶的是,很多当代进化论理论家(其中大多数人自认为是现代进化综论的支持者),最近都成为了‘鲍德温支持者’,这些人是典型的致力于寻找“很难找到的”,群体可以通过行为试错达到人脑、语言和思维快速共同进化的适应性图景之场景的'''进化心理学 Evolutionary Psychology'''家。他们正在寻找被丹尼尔·丹尼特(鲍德温的支持者)所称的“进化起重机”(快速升高重物的工具)。 |
| | | |
| </blockquote> | | </blockquote> |
第235行: |
第236行: |
| According to Daniel Dennett, recent work has rendered the Baldwin effect "no longer a controversial wrinkle in orthodox Darwinism". Potential genetic mechanisms underlying the Baldwin effect have been proposed for the evolution of natural (genetically-determinant) antibodies. In 2009, empirical evidence for the Baldwin effect was provided from the colonisation of North America by the house finch. | | According to Daniel Dennett, recent work has rendered the Baldwin effect "no longer a controversial wrinkle in orthodox Darwinism". Potential genetic mechanisms underlying the Baldwin effect have been proposed for the evolution of natural (genetically-determinant) antibodies. In 2009, empirical evidence for the Baldwin effect was provided from the colonisation of North America by the house finch. |
| | | |
− | 丹尼尔•丹尼特(Daniel Dennett)表示,最近的工作已经使鲍德温效应“在正统达尔文主义中不再是一个有争议的问题”。潜在的遗传机制潜在的鲍德温效应已被提出的进化自然(遗传决定因素)抗体。2009年,经验证明在北美殖民时期为 Baldwin 效应提供了证据。
| + | 丹尼尔•丹尼特表示,最近的研究工作已使鲍德温效应“在正统达尔文主义中不再是争议性问题”。科学家已提出鲍德温效应背后潜在的遗传机制用于自然(基因决定)抗体的进化。2009年,'''家朱雀 House Finch'''在北美的定殖为鲍德温效应提供了经验证明。 |
| | | |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==Comparison with genetic assimilation== | + | ==Comparison with genetic assimilation 与遗传同化的区别== |
| | | |
| {{further|Genetic assimilation}} | | {{further|Genetic assimilation}} |
第249行: |
第250行: |
| The Baldwin effect has been confused with, and sometimes conflated with, a different evolutionary theory also based on phenotypic plasticity, C. H. Waddington's genetic assimilation. The Baldwin effect includes genetic accommodation, of which one type is genetic assimilation.<!--more needed--> | | The Baldwin effect has been confused with, and sometimes conflated with, a different evolutionary theory also based on phenotypic plasticity, C. H. Waddington's genetic assimilation. The Baldwin effect includes genetic accommodation, of which one type is genetic assimilation.<!--more needed--> |
| | | |
− | 鲍德温效应与另一种进化理论相混淆,有时甚至混为一谈,这种进化理论也是基于表型可塑性的基因同化。鲍德温效应包括遗传调节,其中一种类型是遗传同化
| + | 鲍德温效应与另一种同样基于'''表型可塑性 Henotypic Plasticity'''的进化理论——C.H.华丁顿的'''遗传同化 Genetic Assimilation'''相混淆,有时甚至混为一谈。实际上,鲍德温效应包括遗传调节,其中一种类型是遗传同化。 |
| | | |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==See also== | + | ==See also 参见== |
| | | |
| * {{annotated link|Evolvability}} | | * {{annotated link|Evolvability}} |
第269行: |
第270行: |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==Notes== | + | ==Notes 注释== |
| | | |
| {{reflist|30em}} | | {{reflist|30em}} |
第275行: |
第276行: |
| | | |
| | | |
− | == References == | + | == References 参考资料== |
| | | |
| * {{cite journal | last = Bateson |first=Patrick | year = 2004 | title = The Active Role of Behaviour in Evolution | url = | journal = Biology and Philosophy | volume = 19 | issue = 2| pages = 283–298 | doi=10.1023/b:biph.0000024468.12161.83}} | | * {{cite journal | last = Bateson |first=Patrick | year = 2004 | title = The Active Role of Behaviour in Evolution | url = | journal = Biology and Philosophy | volume = 19 | issue = 2| pages = 283–298 | doi=10.1023/b:biph.0000024468.12161.83}} |
第299行: |
第300行: |
| | | |
| | | |
− | ==External links== | + | ==External links 外部链接== |
| | | |
| * [http://www.ecs.soton.ac.uk/~harnad/Hypermail/Cognition.Sociobiology.98/0101.html Baldwinian evolution] | | * [http://www.ecs.soton.ac.uk/~harnad/Hypermail/Cognition.Sociobiology.98/0101.html Baldwinian evolution] |